The relationship of age, early motor skills and observable child behaviors in young children with developmental delays (2024)

  • Journal List
  • HHS Author Manuscripts
  • PMC6739246

As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsem*nt of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice

The relationship of age, early motor skills and observable child behaviors in young children with developmental delays (1)

Link to Publisher's site

Res Dev Disabil. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 Oct 1.

Published in final edited form as:

Res Dev Disabil. 2019 Oct; 93: 103445.

Published online 2019 Jul 31. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2019.103445

PMCID: PMC6739246

NIHMSID: NIHMS1536260

PMID: 31376505

Megan MacDonald1 and Laura Lee McIntyre2

Author information Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

1.0. Introduction

Early childhood is a time of emerging and rapid development (). Noteworthy childhood milestones are typically achieved during this early phase of life and act as building blocks for more advanced skills that develop later in childhood and within multiple domains of development. These milestones might include a child’s first smile, rolling over, laughing, language skills (e.g., first words), sitting up, pulling from sit-to-stand, cruising and walking, to name a few. These developmental milestones are all a part of early learning, which is critical towards the growth of other broader developmental domains including the social, affective, cognitive, motor and physical domains (). Young children typically progress rapidly and sequentially through motor milestones. This includes skills that usually develop within the first year of life such as raising/ holding the head, rolling over, independently sitting and independently walking (which typically occurs around 12 months of age). In the affective domain, children develop healthy attachments to their primary caregivers, and initiate and respond to various emotions including smiling, laughing as well as expressing appropriate negative emotions (e.g., dislike). Young children also begin to cultivate social skills such as functional, imaginative and reciprocal play skills; attentional skills such as compliance and cooperation; and both non-verbal and verbal communication skills.

The rapid development that occurs within early childhood is critical for healthy development early-on and throughout the lifespan (). Various aspects of early childhood development have been deemed critical for later school success and are referred to as school readiness skills (; ). Multiple developmental constructs have been identified to support school readiness, however proficient social skills, communication skills, self-regulation and executive function have been indicated as critical skills for successful school readiness (). As such the child behavior of compliance is an important skill associated with aspects of school readiness. For example, compliance represents a child’s ability to follow instructions and/or rules involved in activities and games. In addition, compliance can be indicative of working memory, as the child is expected to remember the instructions and/or rules involved in games or activities. Empirical research suggests that early childhood providers should focus on improving compliance to assist children with their early self-regulation and school readiness skills (). Adaptive behavior has also been recognized as an important child behavior associated with school readiness outcomes (Finlon et al, 2015; MacDonald, McIntyre, Ross, Tepfer, 2017). Adaptive behavior represents developmentally appropriate behaviors for life circ*mstances, including social competence and a developmentally appropriate level of independence. How other aspects of development facilitate engaging in school readiness skills has been relatively underexplored, including how motor skills may influence aspects of school readiness. When preschoolers were studied over the course of one preschool year, motor skills predicted aspects of school readiness (MacDonald et al., 2016). In short, better motor skills were predictive of more advanced school readiness skills like higher executive function and better social behaviors. Thus, movement and thereby motor skills are important for young children and motor development has implications for other child behaviors highly linked to school readiness.

Children with developmental disabilities often have delays in some developmental domains of early childhood development, and these delays may occur in all or some developmental domains. For example, children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), a known developmental disability, typically experience difficulty with social and communication skills. These delays might include non-verbal or verbal responses and initiations, and difficulty sharing enjoyment, making eye contact with others as well as coordinating gestures appropriately (e.g., linking eye contact, gestures and language together) (Lord et al., 2000; Lord et al., 2012). At the same time, children with ASD have known delays in motor skills. This includes weak postural control, difficulty with balance and delays in achieving early motor milestones (; May et al., 2016; ; ; ; ; Vernazza-Martin et al., 2005). Children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), another developmental disability, have known difficulties with inappropriate attention, impulsiveness and hyperactivity. In addition, about 30– 50% of children with ADHD have known deficits in motor skills (Fliers et al., 2008). Children with speech and language disorders typically have delays in some aspects of speech and/ or non-verbal communication and motor difficulties are also common (Hill, 2001; ). Developmental coordination disorder (DCD), which affects 5– 6% of school aged children is specifically defined based on motor coordination and motor skill deficits. By definition children with DCD do not have another disability that explains their motor skill and/or motor coordination difficulties and these motor difficulties can negatively impact other aspects of their quality of life (). However, DCD is typically not diagnosed until children enter school, therefore it is possible that children with motor skill difficulties may ultimately be diagnosed with DCD at a later age. In brief, motor skill delays experienced by young children with developmental disabilities may put children further behind their peers without disabilities with respect to aspects of early learning, which may ultimately result in weaker school readiness skills.

Although it is known that facets of developmental domains work together, such that there are bidirectional developmental processes occurring simultaneously (Libertus & Needham, 2010; McClelland & Cameron, 2018), it is common for early interventions to be independent from each other in efforts to improve developmental outcomes. For example, an early intervention focused on social behaviors, may specifically target symbolic play (), while another intervention focused on the same overarching domain (social behaviors) may target reciprocal play skills (Dawson et al., 2010). Other early interventions could be focused on other domains of development; for example, motor skill (; ), or speech and language development (), may be targeted without simultaneous intervention efforts in other areas of child development (e.g., social, affective, cognitive). How developmental domains work together in aspects of early development to maximize early learning opportunities is relatively unknown.

Studies focused on specific developmental disabilities have indicated strong relationships between better motor skills and other early learning skills. In a sample of young children with ASD (N= 159) between the ages of 12– 33 months old (1– 2 ½ years), better motor skills were strongly associated with better social communicative skills as indicated through calibrated autism severity scores (). The same research group had similar findings when motor and daily living skills of young children with ASD and other developmental disabilities were examined (N=233) (). In short, better motor skills had strong relationships with other child behavior outcomes including daily living, social and communicative skills (MacDonald et al., 2013). Although strong relationships were indicated, collectively these studies did not tap into underlying mechanisms associated with the potential facilitating role of motor skills and other salient child behaviors associated with school readiness (e.g., executive function, self-regulation, communication and other social behaviors).

A more recent study, focused on young children with developmental disabilities, examined how motor skills mediated relationships between age and the salient child behaviors of socialization, communication and daily living skills in young preschool aged boys (MacDonald et al., 2017). In short better gross motor skills strengthened the relationship between age and socialization, age and communication and age and daily living skills. The authors of this study concluded that content for early intervention needed to include creative learning opportunities and tap into multiple aspects of development including motor skill development. However, in this study the child behavior outcomes of communication, social behavior and daily living skills were based on parent proxy reports (e.g., the Vineland Adaptive Behaviors Scale) and were not directly on observed child behaviors. Although the relationships between better motor skills and other aspects of child development have been studied in young children with developmental disabilities, relationships with direct measures of both motor skills and directly observable child behaviors have not been readily investigated.

Therefore, the purpose of this study was to examine how gross motor skills mediated the relationship of age and compliance as well as age and adaptive behavior in young children (2– 4 years old) with developmental disabilities at one timepoint. More specifically this study aimed to answer the research question “to what extent do early gross motor skills mediate the relationship of age and observed compliance, as well as age and observed adaptive behavior in young children with developmental disabilities at one timepoint”? It was hypothesized that young children’s gross motor skills would positively mediate relationships between age and compliance as well as age and adaptive behavior, in young children with developmental disabilities. This study adds to previous work in that all child behaviors were directly observed (motor skills, compliance and adaptive behavior) in a relatively large sample of young children with confirmed developmental delay and/or developmental disability (N= 113).

2.0. Method

2.1. Procedures

This study was part of a larger investigation of family-based interventions for caregivers of young children with developmental disabilities (funding source added when manuscript is accepted) and was approved by the institutional review board. The current study was a descriptive cross-sectional study with all primary assessments conducted with children and caregivers in the home environment, at one timepoint.

2.2. Participants

Young children (N = 113) were recruited through early intervention programs in the Pacific Northwest region of the United States to participate in the study. Parents/caregivers responded to recruitment flyers that were made available to them through their child’s early intervention provider. After obtaining verbal consent, a research assistant screened parents over the telephone to assess whether their child met the following inclusionary criteria: (a) age between 2 to 3 years (at the time of entrance into the study), (b) documented developmental delay or disability, (c) services received through an Individualized Family Service Plan (IFSP), (d) ambulatory, and (e) living with the primary caregiver for at least 1 year. Exclusion criteria included children who were diagnosed as deaf or blind. For more information please see study XXX.

2.3. Measures

2.3.1. Demographic questionnaire

A research assistant administered a questionnaire during an in-home interview. Information on this questionnaire included child age, gender, race/ethnicity, disability status, special education and related service utilization, parent/caregiver age, education, and household income.

2.3.2. Gross Motor skills

Research assistants administered the Test of Gross Motor Development-2 (TGMD-2; Ulrich, 2000) to participating children. The TGMD-2 is a valid and reliable assessment of gross motor skills for children and has been used frequently with children with disabilities (e.g., Berkeley et al., 2001; MacDonald et al., 2017; ). The TGMD-2 assesses essential skills needed in physical education and active play including locomotor skills (running, galloping, hopping, sliding, leaping, and jumping) and object-control skills (overhand throwing, striking, kicking, underhand rolling, dribbling, and catching). The TGMD-2 is not standardized for children younger than 3 years of age and its easy administration and procedural scoring have been deemed reasonable for children within this age range (MacDonald et al., 2017; ). In addition, raw scores were used in analyses (see data analysis section).

All research assistants were trained to use the TGMD-2. Training was provided to all research assistants by faculty and graduate students who study adapted physical activity/education. Training to administer the TGMD-2 included specific training on the protocol, practice sessions and consensus coding with practiced TGMD-2 administrators.

2.3.3. Observed child behaviors

Observed child behaviors (compliance and adaptive behavior) were coded by trained research assistants during a parent-child interaction task consisting of 10 minutes of unstructured free play and 5 minutes of structured activity. The parent and child dyad were provided with common toys, and the parent was instructed to ‘play as you normally do” during the 10 minute unstructured free play portion. During the final 5 minutes of play a structured activity took place, 2 minutes of “clean-up” (cleaning-up the toys from the free play activity) and 3 minutes of a structured activity (ring-stacking, where the smaller rings were placed on top). The play sessions were video recorded.

2.3.3.1. Compliance

Compliance was scored across the entire 15 minute of the play session, on a 9-point likert scale where 1 represented “not at all” and 9 represented “very much”. Compliance was coded based on answering the following questions: “is the child compliant and cooperative with the parent’s directives and requests?”, “does the child seem dysregulated and difficult to manage and unable to control his/her behavior or emotions?” (reverse coded) and the research assistant categorized overall compliance based on the duration of the observational session. A higher composite score represented more compliance.

2.3.3.2. Adaptive behavior

Adaptive behavior was coded on a 9-point likert scale where 1 represented “not at all” and 9 represented “very much”. Research assistants answered questions based on their impressions of the adaptive behavior of the child during the play session. Questions about adaptive behavior were focused on the child’s ability to adapt, for example, the child’s ability to easily adapt from one activity to another (e.g., cleaning-up after participating in unstructured play, etc). Questions were framed in the negative, thus a lower score represented better adaptive behavior.

The child behaviors of compliance and adaptive behavior were derived from a more comprehensive observation system, which included observations on these child behavior as well as other child behaviors. The inter-rater reliability of the entire observation system was 88%. This was calculated by taking the total number of agreements between raters and dividing by the total number of items on the scale and multiplying by 100 to calculate a percentage.

2.4. Data analysis

All analyses were conducted in SPSS version 22. A bivariate correlation analysis was performed on all variables of interest including the independent variable (age), mediating variable (motor skills), outcome variables (compliance and adaptive behaviors) and potential covariates (e.g., SES, gender). Potential covariates were assessed for inclusion using a planned one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) to test significant effects on the independent, mediating and outcome variables. Two independent simple mediation analyses were conducted using PROCESS, an ordinary least squares path analysis appropriate for small sample sizes (Hayes, 2013). Compared to traditional causal steps mediation analysis (e.g., Baron & Kenny, 1986), PROCESS accounts for violations of the normality assumption and low power issues due to small samples by use of bootstrapping (Hayes, 2013). The magnitude of the indirect effect of motor skills on child development outcomes was tested using standard 95% confidence intervals with a conventional number (10, 000) of bootstrap samples (Hayes, 2013). In each model age was the predictor variable, motor skills were the mediating variable and an observable child behavior variable was the outcome variable (compliance and adaptive behavior, respectively). In the first model, a simple mediation was used to examine the indirect effect of motor skills on relations between age and adaptive behaviors; a single covariate of ethnicity was applied to the adaptive behavior outcome variable based on ANOVA results. In the second model, compliance was the outcome variable of interest. A simple mediation model was used to examine the indirect effect of motor skills on relations between age and compliance; no covariates were included in this mediation analysis, based on non-significant ANOVA analyses. It should be noted that lower scores were representative of more compliance and better adaptive behavior, thus negative correlations and effects were expected. To assist in understanding the effect of each mediation, a proportion of the total effect that was mediated was calculated [ab/(c’+ab)] and translated into percentage ([ab/(c’+ab)]*100).

3.0. Results

Descriptive demographic information about the participants (N= 113) can be found in table 1, descriptive information about mediating and outcome variables can be found in table 2. A bivariate correlation analysis revealed expected significant correlations between predictor (age), mediating (motor skills) and outcome variables (compliance and adaptive behavior). All correlations were interpreted using absolute criterion developed by Zhu (2012), see table 3. All potential covariates were tested using a one-way ANOVA (dependent variables in ANOVAs included both mediating and outcome variables used in the mediation analysis). The only significant covariate was ethnicity, which had a significant relationship with adaptive behavior (F(108)= 3.5, p = 0.01). Thus ethnicity was treated as a single covariate acting on the outcome (Yk) in the adaptive behavior mediation analysis.

Table 1

Descriptive characteristics of the sample (n=113)

VariableN (%)
Gender
 Male84 (74.3)
 Female29 (25.7)
Age (months)M= 45.3, SD= 10.0
Disability (per parental report)
 Developmental delay9 (8)
 Speech/ language delay59( 52.2)
 Autism spectrum disorder16 (14.2)
 Other22 (19.5)
 Unknown1 (0.9)
 Missing6 (5.3)
Ethnicity
 White/ Caucasian74 (65.5)
 Hispanic/ Latino6 (5.3)
 Asian2 (1.8)
 Bi/ multi-racial30 (26.5)
 Other1 (0.9)
Household Income
 <$4, 9991 (.9)
 $5, 000– 9, 99910 (8.8)
 $10, 000– 14, 99918 (15.9)
 $15, 000– 19, 9998 (7.1)
 $20, 000– 24, 9995 (4.4)
 $25, 000– 29, 9998 (7.1)
 $30, 000– 39, 99915 (13.3)
 $40, 000– 49, 99914 (12.4)
 $50, 000– 59, 9998 (7.1)
 $60, 000– 69, 9996 (5.3)
 $70, 000– 79, 9998 (7.1)
 $80, 000– 89, 9996 (5.3)
 $90, 000+6 (5.3)

Open in a separate window

Table 2

Mean score and range for outcome and mediating variables.

VariableNM (SD)Range of scores for observed behaviors
Motor skills
Locomotor raw11321.7 (12.9)0– 77
Object control raw11310.7 (2.8)1– 16
Child behaviors
Compliance1133.0(1.5)1.3– 7
Adaptive1132.3(1.3)1–6.3

Open in a separate window

Table 3

Correlation matrix for primary outcomes, mediating, predicator and potential covariate variables.

Variable1.2.3.4.5.6.7.
1. Compliance--
2. Adaptive behaviors.82***--
3. Motor skills−.31−.34--
4. Age (months)−.26−.39.45**--
5. Annual income.−.07−.13.09.09--
6. Ethnicity.15−.19.12.05−.21--
7. Gender−02−.08.14.11.12.10--

Open in a separate window

Low correlation

*Moderate correlation

**Moderately high correlation

***High correlation (interpretation based on Zhu, 2012)

To examine the extent to which age influenced motor skills and in turn facilitated observed child behavior outcomes of adaptive behavior and compliance two independent mediation analyses were conducted using PROCESS in SPSS (version 22). Represented in model 1 (see table 4) age had positive relations with gross motor skills (a =0.66, p< 0.001), when the outcome variable was adaptive behavior. Similarly, age had positive relations with gross motor skills (a =0.66, p< 0.001), when the outcome variable was compliance (see model 2 in Table 4). Among children demonstrating equivalent motor skills (similar to holding motor skills constant and independent of age) statistically significant differences were observed in adaptive behavior (b=−0.021, p= 0.014) and compliance (b= −0.023, p= 0.020). A bias-corrected 95% bootstrap confidence interval supports a small indirect effect in which age is influencing motor skills, which in turn facilitated better adaptive behaviors (ab = −0.014, 95%CI [(−0.029)- (−0.006)]) and better compliance (ab = −0.015, 95%CI [(−0.036)- (−0.004)]). The proportion of the total effect was translated in percentage; the proportion of the effect that was mediated was 26.4% in model 1 (adaptive behavior) and 40.5% in model 2 (compliance).

Table 4

Results from two independent simple mediation analyses (model 1–2) for the effect of age (X) on child development domains: adaptive behaviors (Y1), compliance (Y2) through motor skill competency (M). N=113

Outcome
Model 1M (Total Motor)Y1 (Adaptive)
  PredictorCoeff.Std. Errorp [95% CI]Coeff.Std. Errorp [95% CI]
  X (Age)a .664.127<000***c’ −.039.012.002
  M (Total Motor)---------b −.021.008.014
  C (Ethnicity)---------g .100.036.007
  Constanti1 2.2735.876.700i2 4.407.522.000***
  Indirect Effectab −.014.006[−.0293– −.0053]Proportion Mediation (%)=26.4
R2= 0.199R2= 0.238
F (1, 111) = 27.54, p<.000***F (3, 109)= 11.39, p= .000
M (Total Motor)Y2 (Compliance)
Model 2
  X (Age)a .664.127<000***c’ −.022.015.126
  M (Total Motor)---------b −.023.010.020*
  Constanti2 2.2735.876.700i3 4.752.607.000
  Indirect Effectab −.015.008[−.036– −.004]Proportion mediation (%)=40.5
R2= .446R2= .3356
F (1, 111)= 27.54, p< .000F (2, 110)= 6.98, p=.001

Open in a separate window

NOTE: Results for indirect effect (ab), Preacher & Kelley (2011) kappa (k2) and associated std. error and bias-corrected confidence intervals (95%CI) based on 10,000 bootstrap sample

*p< 0.05

**p<0.01

***p<0.001 two tailed

4.0. Discussion

The results of this study support the facilitating role of gross motor skills in the relationship of chronological age and observed compliance and observed adaptive behavior in young children with developmental disabilities, at one timepoint. It is well known that children with developmental disabilities are at risk for delays in childhood development (especially in early childhood), and that delays may be present in one or more domains. Yet, how various developmental domains work together has had less attention in empirical research. From a school readiness perspective, motor skill and physical development have been indicated as components of school readiness (e.g., health and motor skill development), but less attention has been given to these aspects of development compared to others (e.g., socio-emotional development, motivation to learn, language and early literacy skills and conception knowledge and application). In one recent study, the strength of the relationship between motor skills and other constructs of school readiness were stronger in children with disabilities, and the authors suggested amplified benefits of motor skill development for children with disabilities (). For example, better motor skills may provide opportunities for functional play (e.g., climbing on the jungle gym), and these functional play opportunities may allow for social behaviors and communication skills to be practiced alongside peers (e.g., showing a peer a climbing task on the jungle gym). In addition, motor skills are used in the act of gesturing, an act used in communication. Thus, better motor skills for young children with disabilities may help to compensate for delays in some of these other areas, while simultaneously allowing practice time (e.g., playing alongside a peer in the school yard, and unknowingly practicing reciprocal aspects of communication). Furthermore, previous work indicates that motor skills are malleable (; ). Thus, functional motor skills and be taught, practiced and reinforced in everyday childhood activities.

Early childhood is a key developmental period to work on improving known delays, and ultimately work toward closing the developmental gap between children with developmental disabilities and their peers without disabilities. Early intervention contributes efforts towards improving known delays. In the past two decades, substantial effort in early childhood research has focused on national priorities with an underlying theme of school readiness (Panel, 1995). In the US, kindergarten is the entry point for children to enter formal education, where children begin the formal process of academic learning. Prior to entering the education system, children need to develop a skillset that prepares them for school. Domains within school readiness initiatives include 1) physical well-being and motor development, 2) social and emotional development, 3) approaches to learning, 4) language development and 5) cognition and general knowledge (Panel, 1995). Although all five domains have been indicated to be important within school readiness initiatives research has primarily focused on all aforementioned domains, except for physical well-being and motor development. Yet, the results of this study suggest that better motor skills may influence the relationship between age and the observed child behavior outcomes of compliance (social and emotional development) and adaptive behavior (cognition and general knowledge) in young children with developmental disabilities.

Common salient child behaviors like compliance and adaptive behavior improve with age, thus a chronological developmental sequence occurs, typically with substantial gains in early childhood. The results of this study suggest that motor skills influence the relationship of age and compliance and the relationship of age and adaptive behavior. Thus early learning interventions related to social and emotional development (e.g., compliance), cognition and general knowledge (e.g., adaptive behavior), could also benefit from including specific developmental content focused on motor skill development (from the physical well-being and motor development domain).

Children with developmental delays have known deficits early on, especially in school readiness skills. The importance of play to engage and hone-in on early learning skills has been identified as a critical way to engage young children in developmental outcomes. Motor skill development has functional play implications, thus better motor skills may promote other child developmental outcomes, while poorer motor skills may hinder these outcomes. The results of this study suggest developmental domains are interactive and thus improvements in one domain, like motor skill development, may ultimately have positive implications for other developmental domains, including better school readiness skills.

Finally, this study is not without limitations. For example, it is possible that participants who were more compliant during free play were also more compliant participating in the motor skill assessment. This study was a cross-sectional descriptive study, thus no longitudinal or causal relationships can be established.

5.0. Conclusion

The results of this study indicate that motor skills influence the relationship between age and compliance and the relationship of age and adaptive behavior in young children with developmental disabilities, at one time point. In the field of adapted physical activity, there have been successful interventions when teaching motor skills to young children with disabilities (Bremer et al., 2014; Ketcheson et al., 2016). Thus including motor skill development as a part of early learning initiatives, especially for young children with developmental disabilities, may positively impact positively motor skill development and other important child behaviors.

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by a grant from the National Institutes of Health (R01 HD059838) awarded to the second author.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • Blair C & Raver CC (2015). School readiness and self-regulation: A developmental psychobiological approach. Annual review of psychology, 66, 711–731. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015221 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Bremer E, Balogh R, & Lloyd M (2014). Effectiveness of a fundamental motor skill intervention for 4-year old children with autism spectrum disorder: A pilot study. Autism, Epub ahead of print. doi: 10.1177/1362361314557548 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Dawson G, Rogers S, Munson J, Smith M, Winter J, Greenson J, … Varley J (2010). Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention for infants with autism: The Early Start Denver Model. Pediatrics, 125(1), 17–23. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-0958. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Fliers E, Rommelse N, Vermeulen S, Altink M, Buschgens C, Faraone S, & Buitelaar J (2008). Motor coordination problems in children and adolescents with ADHS rated by parents and teachers: Effects of age and gender. Journal of neural transmission, 115(2), 211–220. doi: 10.1007/s00702-007-0827-0 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Gehris JS, Gooze RA, & Whitaker RC (2015). Teachers’ perceptions about children’s movment and learning in early childhood education programmes. Child: Care, health and development, 41(1), 122–131. doi: 10.1111/cch.12136 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Goodway J, & Branta C (2003). Influence of motor skill intervention on fundamental motor skill development of disadvantaged preschool children. Research quarterly in exercise and sport, 74(1), 36–46. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2003.10609062 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Goodway J, Crowe H, & Ward P (2003). Effects of motor skill instruction on fundamental motor skill development. Adapted phyiscal activity quarterly, 20(3), 298–314. [Google Scholar]
  • Hampton LH, & Kaiser AP (2016). Intervention effects on spoken-language outcomes for children with autism: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of intellectual disability research, 60(5), 444–463. doi: 10.1111/jir.12283 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Hayes A (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guildford Press. [Google Scholar]
  • Hill E (2001). Non-specific nature of specific language impaimrent: A review of literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. International journal of language and communication disorders, 46, 149–171. doi: 10.1080/13682820010019874 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Houwen S, Visser S, van der Putten A, & Vlaskamp C (2016). The interrelationship between motor, cognitive, and language development in children with and without intellectual and developmental disabilities. Research in developmental disabilities, Epub ahead of print, 19–31. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2016.01.012 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kasari C, Freeman S, & Paparella T (2006). Joint attention and symbolic play in young children with autism: a randomized controlled intervention study. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 47(6), 611–620. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01567.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kasari C, Freeman S, Paperella T, Wong C, Kwon S, & Gultrud A (2005). Early intervention of core deficits in autism. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 2(6), 380–388. [Google Scholar]
  • Ketcheson L, Hauck J, & Ulrich D (2016). The effects of an early motor skill intervention on the motor skills, levels of physical activity, and socialization in young children with autism spectrum disorder: A pilot study. Autism, Epub ahead of print1–12. doi: 10.1177/136236131660611 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kim H, Carlson A, Curby T, & Winsler A (2016). Relations among motor, social, and cognitive skills in pre-kindergarten children with developmental disabilities. Research in developmental disabilities, [Epub ahead of print], 43–60. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2016.01.016 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Libetus K & Hauf P (2017). Editorial: Motor skills and their foundational role for perceptual, social, and cognitive development. Frontiers in psychology. 8(301). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00301. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Lloyd M, MacDonald M, & Lord C (2013). Motor skills of toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 17(2), 133–146. doi: 10.1177/1362361311402230 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook E, Leventhal B, DiLavore P, … Rutter M (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule- Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore P, Risi S, Gotham K, & Bishop S (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule, second edition (ADOS-2) manual (part 1): Modules1–4. Torrence, CA: Western Psychologicl Services. [Google Scholar]
  • MacDonald M, Lipscomb S, McClelland M, Duncan R, Becker D, & Kile M (2016). Preschoolers’ fine and gross motor skills differentially predict executive function and social behavior. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2016.12229862 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • MacDonald M, Lord C, & Ulrich D (2013). The relationship of motor skills and adaptive behavior skills in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in autism spectrum disorders, 7(11), 1383–1390. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2013.07.020 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • MacDonald M, Lord C, & Ulrich D (2014). Motor skills and calibrated autism severity in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 95–105. doi: 10.1123/apaq.2013.0068 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • MacDonald M, McIntyre L, Ross S, & Tepfer A (2017). Relations of early motor skills on age socialization, communication, and daily living in young children with developmental disabilities. Adapted phyiscal activity quarterly, 34(2), 179–194. doi: 10.1123/apaq.2015-0091 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • May T, McGinley J, Murphy A, Hinkley T, Papadopoulos N, Williams KJ, … Rinehard N (2016). A multidisciplinary perspective on motor impairments as an early behavioural marker in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autralian psychologist, 51(4), 296–303. doi: 10.1111/ap.12225 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • McClelland M, Aco*ck A, & Morrison F (2006). The impact of kindergarten learning-related skills on academic trajectories at the end of elementary school. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 21, 471–490. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2006.09.003 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • McClelland MM, & Cameron CE (2011). Self-regulation in early childhood: Improving conceptual clarity and developing ecologically valid measures. Child Development Perspectives, 6(2), 136–142. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00191.x [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • McClelland MM, & Cameron CE (2011). Developing together: The role of executive function and motor skills in chilren’s early academic lives. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, Epub ahead of print. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.03.014. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Pagani LS, & Messier S (2012). Links between motor skills and indicators of school readiness at kindergarten entry in urban disadvantaged children. Journal of educational and developmental psychology, 2(1), 95–107. doi: 10.5539/jedp.v2n1p95 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Panel NEG (1995). Reconsidering children’s early development and learning: Toward common views and vocabulary. Retrieved from http://govinfo.library.unt.edu/negp/reports/child-ea.htm
  • Phillips D, & Shonkoff J (Eds.). (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood developmentWashinton, DC: National Academies Press. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Pratt ME, Lipscomb ST & McClelland MM (2016). Caregiver responsiveness during preschool supports cooperation in Kindergarten: Moderation by chidlren’s early compliance. Early Education and Development, 27(4), 421–439. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2016.1090767. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Provost B, Heimerl S, & Lopez BR (2007). Levels of gross and fine motor development in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Physical & Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 27(3). Doi: 10.1080/J006v27n03_03 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Provost B, Lopez BR, & Heimerl S (2007). A comparison in motor delays in young children: Autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and developmental concerns. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 321–328. Doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0170-6. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Sparrow S, Cicchetti D, & Balla D (2005). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition (2nd ed.): Pearson. [Google Scholar]
  • Staples K, & Reid G (2010). Fundamental movement skills and autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 40(2), 209–217. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0854-9 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Teitelbaum P, Teitelbaum O, Nye J, Fryman J, & Maurer RG (1998). Movement analysis in infancy may be useful for early diagnosis of autism. Psychology, 95, 13982–13987. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Vernazza-Martin S, Martin N, Vernazza A, Lepellec-Muller A, Rufo M, Massion J, & Assaiante C (2005). Goal directed locomotion and balance control in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(1), 91–102. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Webster R, Majnemer A, Platt R, & Shevell M (2005). Motor function at school age in children with a preschool diagnosis of developmetnal language impairment. The journal of pediatrics, 146, 80–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2004.09.005 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Zhu W (2012). Sadly, the earth is still round (p < 0.05). Journal of sport and health science, 1, 9–11. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2012.02.002 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Zwicker JG, Suto M & Harris SR (2017). Developmental coordination disorder is more than a motor problem: Children describe the ipmact of daily struggles on their quality of life. British journal of occupational therapy, 81(2), 65–73. Doi: 10.1177/0308022617735046. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
The relationship of age, early motor skills and observable child behaviors in young children with developmental delays (2024)
Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Kieth Sipes

Last Updated:

Views: 6261

Rating: 4.7 / 5 (67 voted)

Reviews: 82% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Kieth Sipes

Birthday: 2001-04-14

Address: Suite 492 62479 Champlin Loop, South Catrice, MS 57271

Phone: +9663362133320

Job: District Sales Analyst

Hobby: Digital arts, Dance, Ghost hunting, Worldbuilding, Kayaking, Table tennis, 3D printing

Introduction: My name is Kieth Sipes, I am a zany, rich, courageous, powerful, faithful, jolly, excited person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.